Skip to main content
  • Original article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Estimated intracranial volume from FreeSurfer is biased by total brain volume

Abstract

Background

Estimated intracranial volume (eTIV) from FreeSurfer is not segmentation-based but calculated from the alignment of the input magnetic resonance (MR) images to the MNI305 brain atlas, an approach that could lead to a bias by total brain volume. If eTIV is unbiased, variance beyond that explained by intracranial volume should be random. Our null hypothesis was that no correlation would remain between eTIV and total brain volume when controlling for intracranial volume.

Methods

eTIV and total brain volume for 62 participants were calculated on 1.5-T, T1-weighted MR images using FreeSurfer (version 6.0.0). Manual delineations of the intracranial volume were also made for the same images. To evaluate the null hypothesis, the partial correlation between eTIV and total brain volume was calculated when controlling for intracranial volume.

Results

The partial correlation between eTIV and total brain volume when controlling for intracranial volume was 0.355 (p = 0.026). The null hypothesis was rejected.

Conclusion

eTIV from FreeSurfer is biased by total brain volume.

Key points

  • eTIV from FreeSurfer is not calculated by delineation of the dura mater

  • eTIV is biased by total brain volume

  • eTIV might be suboptimal for intracranial volume normalisation

Background

Intracranial volume is frequently estimated using the software suite FreeSurfer and is then commonly abbreviated as eTIV (estimated total intracranial volume) [1]. eTIV is automatically calculated by dividing a predetermined constant with the factor by which the input magnetic resonance (MR) images are scaled in size to align to the MNI305 head atlas [2,3,4]. The predetermined constant and the intracranial volume of the MR images are assumed to be similar to each other after the alignment, so division by the scaling factor should give a fair estimate of the intracranial volume before the alignment. The calculation of eTIV originates from a method described by Buckner et al. [5]. eTIV estimated by FreeSurfer has previously been reported to have Pearson correlations of 0.89–0.94 with manually estimated intracranial volume [6,7,8].

The alignment optimisation of the input MR images to the MNI305 brain atlas is done using Avi Snyder’s 4dfp suite of image registration tools [2, 9]. The resulting twelve-parameter affine transformation matrix tells the scaling, rotation, shearing, and translation of the aligned MR images in the three spatial dimensions, and its determinant gives the total factor by which the images were scaled during the alignment [3, 9, 10]. eTIV is then calculated by a predetermined constant (1948 mL) divided by the scaling factor [3, 11]. The predetermined constant is used to get a volumetric measure from the otherwise dimensionless scaling factor and was obtained as the slope of a linear regression without the intercept term, between manually estimated intracranial volume and corresponding scaling factors in 22 cases [3].

A potential problem of eTIV is that it is derived by the alignment of intensities not only in the skull, but also in the cerebrospinal fluid, fat, and brain tissue [12]. If a flawless alignment of brain tissues alone was done, the resulting scaling factor would be equal to the total brain volume of the atlas divided by the total brain volume of the MR images. Then, as both the predetermined constant and the brain volume of the atlas are constants, eTIV would become proportional to the total brain volume of the MR images and, therefore, depending on brain atrophy. The question is if the additional contribution of non-brain voxels to the alignment removes such a bias.

If eTIV is not biased, its variance should be random beyond that explained by the true intracranial volume. Our null hypothesis is that no correlation will remain between eTIV and total brain volume when controlling for fully delineated intracranial volume.

Methods

Thirty-three memory clinic patients and 29 healthy controls were included from the Gothenburg MCI study [13, 14]. These participants had already been included in two previous studies about manual estimation of intracranial volume [15, 16]. The patients were classified as having either subjective cognitive impairment, mild cognitive impairment or probable dementia by the global deterioration scale for assessing cognitive impairment [14, 17]. Demographics are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographics

For each participant, MR images acquired with a three-dimensional, T1-weighted, magnetisation-prepared, rapid gradient echo (inversion recovery/gradient recalled) sequence on a 1.5-T Symphony scanner (Siemens Healthineers, Erlangen, Germany) were available. The acquisition parameters were as follows: inversion time 820 ms; repetition time 1610 ms; echo time 2.38 ms; flip angle 15°; field of view 250 × 203 mm; matrix 512 × 416; acquisition pixel spacing 1.0 × 1.0, reconstruction pixel spacing 0.49 × 0.49 mm; slice thickness 1 mm; spacing between slices 1 mm (no interslice gap); receiver bandwidth 220 Hz/pixel; number of slices 192; acquisition time 1.7–2.4 min; body transmit coil type.

Manually estimated intracranial volume

The manual estimates of intracranial volume were included from a previous study [15] where the same participants and MR images were used. In the previous study, the intracranial volumes were estimated by delineating all sagittal intracranial areas in the MR images following the protocol described by Eritaia et al. [18].

FreeSurfer estimates

eTIV and total brain volumes were calculated using FreeSurfer version 6.0.0 run on a MacPro 3.1 with two quad-core Intel Xeon 2.8-GHz, 64-bit processors, 8 GB of RAM, and Mac OS X version 10.8.5 (Apple Inc., Cupertino, CA, USA). The brain volume estimation in FreeSurfer is an automatic classification of voxels into brain tissue labels, described by Fischl et al. [19].

Visual inspection of the alignments from which eTIV was calculated did not reveal any obvious errors, so no exclusion because of alignment errors was performed. Visual inspection of the brain volume segmentations revealed partial inclusion of dura mater, missed voxels of cortex, partial inclusion of sinuses, and, in some cases, partial inclusion of porous bone. We did correct for these errors following the guidelines [20]. The error checking and corrections were done blinded to participant age, gender, and cognitive status.

Statistical analysis

The median partial correlation [21] between eTIV and total brain volume when controlling for intracranial volume was calculated using delete-two Jackknife resampling [22, 23]. The null hypothesis was then tested using the Jackknife replicate with the lowest partial correlation to assure that no pair of data points had an exceptionally large impact on the partial correlation. The alpha value was set to 0.05. By simulation (Additional file 1), the risk of a false positive finding in the current setting should be about 5% or less, assuming that the manual estimates of intracranial volume have a Pearson correlation of 0.99 or higher to actual intracranial volume. Further, Pearson correlations were calculated between intracranial volume, eTIV, and total brain volume. All statistics were performed in MATLAB version R2015b (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA).

Results

The Jackknife replicate with the lowest partial correlation between eTIV and total brain volume when controlling for intracranial volume showed a significant correlation of 0.290 (p = 0.026). The median partial correlation for all Jackknife replicates was 0.355. Thus, about 13% (≈ 0.3552 x 100) of the variance in eTIV that was not explained by intracranial volume was explained by total brain volume.

The Pearson correlation coefficient between intracranial volume and eTIV was 0.960 for all participants, 0.954 for the controls, and 0.971 for participants with probable dementia. The Pearson correlation coefficient between intracranial volume and total brain volume was 0.921 for all participants, 0.934 for the controls, and 0.919 for participants with probable dementia. The Pearson correlation coefficient between eTIV and total brain volume was 0.923 for all participants, 0.928 for the controls, and 0.921 for participants with probable dementia.

On average, eTIV overestimated the intracranial volume by 4% (Table 1).

Discussion

Our study found a significant partial correlation between eTIV and total brain volume when controlling for intracranial volume, so the null hypothesis was rejected. This finding implies that eTIV is biased by total brain volume and reinforces the doubts about eTIV construct validity. Unfortunately, the strong collinearity between intracranial volume and total brain volume makes it difficult to determine the exact extent of the bias.

Two longitudinal studies have previously evaluated the possibility of a total-brain-volume-dependent bias in eTIV estimation. To assess the bias, both studies calculated the Pearson correlation coefficient between the change in total brain volume and the change in eTIV over time [7, 24]. One of these studies found a Pearson correlation coefficient of 0.515 (p = 0.05, n = 11) when using FreeSurfer version 3.0.2 in participants with frontal lobe dementia [24]. The other study did not see any tendency towards a significant Pearson correlation when using FreeSurfer version 5.1.0 in healthy elderly (p = 0.892, r = − 0.019, n = 53) [7]. The small number of participants in the first of these studies might have contributed to the non-significant finding. In the second study, an increase in both the manual estimates of intracranial volume and eTIV was seen between baseline and follow-up. According to Nordenskjöld et al. [7], these volume increases might have been the effect of a system upgrade of the MR scanner, which could have interfered with the results.

Besides the use of different study designs, an explanation to why a total-brain-volume-dependent bias was seen in the present study and indicated by Pengas et al. [24], but not by Nordenskjöld et al. [7], could be the use of different participant groups. The MNI305 atlas is based on healthy young adults and the atlas brain has fairly small lateral ventricles. With larger lateral ventricles in the images to analyse, the ventricular volume might act to increase the size of the atlas during the alignment, thus counteracting the effect of cortical atrophy on eTIV. Further, cortical atrophy that only widens the sulci will likely have less impact on the alignment with regard to a total-brain-volume-dependent bias, as parts of the outer border of the brain still lies close to the dura mater. Conversely, a cortical atrophy that locally or globally retracts the gyri in combination with less ventricular enlargement might result in the brain-volume-dependent-bias to become more apparent. This could explain why Pengas et al. [24] almost found a significant bias though only following 11 participants; all of them had frontal lobe dementia. It could also explain why only a small bias was found in the present study where both healthy elderly and patients with different dementia diseases were included, and could help explaining why no bias was detected by Nordenskjöld et al. [7].

The Pearson correlation between eTIV and intracranial volume is stronger in the present study (r = 0.96) than in previously published studies. The Pearson correlation coefficient between eTIV and manually estimated intracranial volume typically ranges between 0.89 and 0.94 [6,7,8]. The mean percentage error in eTIV in the dementia group in the present study is similar to that found by Malone et al. (+ 3.7%) [8], who evaluated eTIV estimated from 288 participants with probable Alzheimer’s disease. In the study by Nordenskjöld et al. [7], FreeSurfer also tended to overestimate intracranial volume, but in spite of the possible scanner drift, the average overestimation of eTIV decreased with age. According to the expected total-brain-volume-dependent bias in eTIV, eTIV should reduce with atrophy. This is also the case in the present study where the overestimation was smaller in the dementia group (+ 3.6%) compared to the control group (+ 4.4%).

In a study by Hansen et al. [25], normalisation by eTIV decreased the sample sizes needed to detect a volume difference in hippocampal volume between two hypothetical groups. Linear normalisation by eTIV even outperformed linear normalisation using more valid estimates. However, it seems to be assumed that the mean volume difference between the two groups will not be affected by normalisation. Under this assumption, the smallest sample sizes will be achieved using intracranial volume estimates that explain the most variance in the volume of interest. The additional variance that eTIV explains compared to the more valid estimates risks being variance due to the total-brain-volume-dependent bias, which in reality might reduce the mean volume difference between the two groups during normalisation, thus reducing the gain of the normalisation.

Voevodskaya et al. [26] have evaluated ratio and linear regression normalisation when using eTIV from FreeSurfer version 5.1.0. In their study, there was only a very slight advantage in classification performance between controls, participants with mild cognitive impairment, and patients with Alzheimer’s disease when using hippocampal volumes normalised by eTIV instead of raw hippocampal volumes. The combined impression from two studies by Westman et al. [27] and Zhou et al. [28] is that ratio normalisation with eTIV is not beneficial for multivariate classification of controls and patients with Alzheimer’s disease, and questionable for univariate classification models. The small benefit of eTIV normalisation in these three studies could be due to a number of reasons, such as: (1) the total-brain-volume-dependent bias in eTIV; (2) the choice of normalisation method; and (3) the reduced need of normalisation when comparing groups with large mean volume differences.

In studies where total brain volume loss is small, the bias in eTIV will be small too, but even then it is probably better to use manual estimates of intracranial volume or even the total brain volume estimate from FreeSurfer. Lehmann et al. [29] report a Pearson correlation coefficient of r ≥ 0.98 between manually estimated total brain volume and the total brain volume estimate from FreeSurfer, a correlation stronger than those reported between eTIV and manual estimates of intracranial volume [6,7,8]. Thus, for samples with small total brain volume loss, total brain volume should have a better chance to reduce variance explained by premorbid total brain volume than eTIV has.

When troubleshooting the output from the FreeSurfer analyses, it is recommended to inspect the atlas alignment and correct it if necessary. The instructions for manual correction of the atlas alignment states that: “The goal is to stretch, translate, and rotate your moveable volume so that the two brains look as similar as possible, at least along the key anatomical points (anterior/posterior commissures, the temporal lobes in the coronal plane, and the midline cut)” [10]. The two brains mentioned in the instruction are those from the input MR images and the atlas (the movable volume). Following these instructions, there is a risk that eTIV is made to depend even more on the brain, as the alignment of the intracranial cavity or the skull is not considered.

Besides FreeSurfer, other automatic approaches for intracranial volume estimation exist. While these methods produce estimates with correlations between 0.86 and 0.99 to manually estimated intracranial volumes [7, 8, 30, 31], they too need more thorough evaluation. For now, manual estimation by the delineation of the dura mater is the safest way to obtain valid intracranial volume estimates in T1-weighted MR imaging. Just by delineating two selected intracranial areas, estimates with Pearson correlation coefficients to fully delineated intracranial volumes above 0.99 may be achieved [16]. The delineation of the dura mater minimises the risk of bias by brain morphology or total brain volume.

The present study has limitations. Our interpretation of the rejected null hypothesis assumes that the manually delineated intracranial volume captures most of the variance of the actual intracranial volume. If this would not be true, the correlation between eTIV and total brain volume that is not explained by the intracranial volume estimates could be due to erroneously disregarded variance. However, the estimation approach behind eTIV, our use of fully delineated intracranial volume and the well-defined dura mater in the MR images (see Fig. 1) make such an explanation improbable. In addition, the exact extent of total-brain-volume-dependent bias in eTIV cannot be determined using the methodology of the present study and remains an issue for further investigations. Finally, we note that another issue for further investigation is how the total-brain-volume-dependent bias in eTIV varies with the use of MR scans with different acquisition parameters, field strengths, and scanner manufacturers.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Illustration of FreeSurfer atlas alignment for estimation of intracranial volume. a T1-weighted image of a participant with frontal lobe dementia. b MNI305 atlas aligned to the T1-weighted image. c T1-weighted image with the aligned atlas overlaid and the intracranial surface of the atlas delineated (pink). d T1-weighted image with the brain surface (blue contour), the intracranial surface of the atlas (pink), and the actual intracranial surface (green) delineated; the atlas surface (pink) seems to follow an outer perimeter set by the gyri rather than following the intracranial surface (green)

In conclusion, we showed that eTIV from FreeSurfer is biased by total brain volume. Before more thorough evaluations or methodological improvements of eTIV become available, the use of eTIV in normalisation of regional brain volume should be considered with care.

Abbreviations

eTIV:

Estimated total intracranial volume from FreeSurfer

MR:

Magnetic resonance

References

  1. Fischl B (2012) FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 62:774–781

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. talairach_avi. https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/talairach_avi. Accessed 1 Feb 2016

  3. eTIV. http://www.freesurfer.net/fswiki/eTIV. Accessed 24 Jan 2018

  4. Evans AC, Collins DL, Mills SR, Brown ED, Kelly RL, Peters TM (1993) 3d statistical neuroanatomical models from 305 MRI volumes. Nucl Sci Symp Med Imaging Conf 1-3:1813–1817. https://doi.org/10.1109/NSSMIC.1993.373602

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Buckner RL, Head D, Parker J et al (2004) A unified approach for morphometric and functional data analysis in young, old, and demented adults using automated atlas-based head size normalization: reliability and validation against manual measurement of total intracranial volume. Neuroimage 23:724–738

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Shen L, Saykin AJ, Kim S et al (2010) Comparison of manual and automated determination of hippocampal volumes in MCI and early AD. Brain Imaging Behav 4:86–95

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Nordenskjöld R, Malmberg F, Larsson EM et al (2013) Intracranial volume estimated with commonly used methods could introduce bias in studies including brain volume measurements. Neuroimage 83:355–360

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Malone IB, Leung KK, Clegg S et al (2015) Accurate automatic estimation of total intracranial volume: a nuisance variable with less nuisance. Neuroimage 104:366–372

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. talairach. https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/talairach. Accessed 24 Jan 2018

  10. Fixing bad output from the Talairach registration. http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/FsTutorial/Talairach_freeview. Accessed 24 Jan 2018

  11. Release Notes. https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/PreviousReleaseNotes. Accessed 24 Jan 2018

  12. What is included in the ICV value. https://mail.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/pipermail/freesurfer/2008-September/008370.html. Accessed 24 Jan 2018

  13. Wallin A, Nordlund A, Jonsson M et al (2016) Alzheimer’s disease-subcortical vascular disease spectrum in a hospital-based setting: overview of results from the Gothenburg MCI and dementia studies. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 36:95–113

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Wallin A, Nordlund A, Jonsson M et al (2016) The Gothenburg MCI study: design and distribution of Alzheimer’s disease and subcortical vascular disease diagnoses from baseline to 6-year follow-up. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 36:114–131

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Klasson N, Olsson E, Rudemo M, Eckerström C, Malmgren H, Wallin A (2015) Valid and efficient manual estimates of intracranial volume from magnetic resonance images. BMC Med Imaging 15:5

  16. Klasson N, Olsson E, Eckerström C, Malmgren H, Wallin A (2018) Delineation of two intracranial areas and the perpendicular intracranial width is sufficient for intracranial volume estimation. Insights imaging 9:25–34

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Reisberg B, Ferris SH, de Leon MJ, Crook T (1988) Global Deterioration Scale (GDS). Psychopharmacol Bull 24:661–663

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Eritaia J, Wood SJ, Stuart GW et al (2000) An optimized method for estimating intracranial volume from magnetic resonance images. Magn Reson Med 44:973–977

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E et al (2002) Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33:341–355

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Editing the aseg. https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/TkMeditGuide/TkMeditWorkingWithData/FreeviewSegmentations. Accessed 18 Apr 2018

  21. Altman DG (1991) Practical statistics for medical research. Chapman and Hall, London

    Google Scholar 

  22. Pigeot I (2001) The jackknife and bootstrap in biomedical research—Common principles and possible pitfalls. Therapeutic Innovation & Regulatory Science 35:1431–1443

    Google Scholar 

  23. Shao J, Wu CFJ (1989) A general theory for Jackknife variance estimation. Ann Statist 17:1176–1197. https://doi.org/10.1214/aos/1176347263

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Pengas G, Pereira JM, Williams GB, Nestor PJ (2009) Comparative reliability of total intracranial volume estimation methods and the influence of atrophy in a longitudinal semantic dementia cohort. J Neuroimaging 19:37–46

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Hansen TI, Brezova V, Eikenes L, Håberg A, Vangberg TR (2015) How does the accuracy of intracranial volume measurements affect normalized brain volumes? Sample size estimates based on 966 subjects from the HUNT MRI cohort. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 36:1450–1456

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Voevodskaya O, Simmons A, Nordenskjöld R et al (2014) The effects of intracranial volume adjustment approaches on multiple regional MRI volumes in healthy aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Front Aging Neurosci 6:264

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Westman E, Aguilar C, Muehlboeck JS, Simmons A (2013) Regional magnetic resonance imaging measures for multivariate analysis in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Brain Topogr 26:9–23

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Zhou Q, Goryawala M, Cabrerizo M, Barker W, Duara R, Adjouadi M (2014) Significance of normalization on anatomical MRI measures in predicting Alzheimer’s disease. ScientificWorldJournal 2014:541802

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Lehmann M, Douiri A, Kim LG et al (2010) Atrophy patterns in Alzheimer's disease and semantic dementia: a comparison of FreeSurfer and manual volumetric measurements. Neuroimage 49:2264–2274

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Keihaninejad S, Heckemann RA, Fagiolo G, Symms MR, Hajnal JV, Hammers A (2010) A robust method to estimate the intracranial volume across MRI field strengths (1.5T and 3T). Neuroimage 50:1427–1437

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Ishii K, Soma T, Kono AK et al (2006) Automatic volumetric measurement of segmented brain structures on magnetic resonance imaging. Radiat Med 24:422–430

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Availability of data and materials

Requests to access the imaging data or the software used for manual delineations should be sent to the corresponding author. FreeSurfer was freely available for download at the time of publication (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/).

Funding

The present study was funded by contributions from Hjalmar Svensson Foundation (HJSV2012032, HJSV201348), the Gothenburg Foundation for Neurological Research (Insamlingsstiftelsen för neurologisk forskning), Wilhelm och Martina Lundgrens Vetenskapsfond (vet1-284/2013), the Swedish Research Council (2014-5182-117745-36), and the Sahlgrenska University Hospital.

Acknowledgements

Thanks to colleagues at the Memory Clinic in Mölndal, MedTech West, and the Department of Radiology in Mölndal.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

NK conceived and designed the study, did the manual delineations of the intracranial volumes, performed the FreeSurfer analyses, performed the statistical analyses, and drafted the manuscript. Dr. EO and Professor emeritus HM helped to design the study and to draft the manuscript. Dr. CE and Professor emeritus AW helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Niklas Klasson.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The present study is a substudy of the Gothenburg MCI study. All participants provided written informed consent to participate in the Gothenburg MCI study. The Gothenburg MCI study complies with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the Ethical Review Board in Gothenburg has approved the study (diary number: L091-99, 1999, T479-11, 2011).

Consent for publication

Not applicable

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interest.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Simulation script that evaluates the delete-two Jackknife resampling approach that was used in the partial correlation analysis of the present study. The simulation evaluates the risk of getting a false positive finding if there is no total-brain-volume-dependent bias in estimated total intracranial volume from FreeSurfer. The different parameters in the MATLAB script may be changed to see how the risk of getting a false positive finding changes. The current parameters were set to resemble those of the present study. (M 12 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Klasson, N., Olsson, E., Eckerström, C. et al. Estimated intracranial volume from FreeSurfer is biased by total brain volume. Eur Radiol Exp 2, 24 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41747-018-0055-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41747-018-0055-4

Keywords